How Should We Learn about Cultural Differences?

trainingIntegral to the global business landscape is intercultural contact and communication. As such, one of the basic requirements for people to successfully deal with this diverse landscape is cultural sensitivity, or cultural intelligence (CQ). Be it international student-trainees, temporary expatriates, permanent migrants, or local workers of a multinational company, we would want these people to understand cultural differences and to adjust their behaviors and communication style according to these differences. Thus, whatever the form of intercultural contact, learning about cultures becomes essential. Given this, decades of research and corporate practice have promoted and celebrated different forms of cultural education and training. Yet,

Is learning about cultural differences necessarily always positive?

A recent research study by Emma Buchtel (2014) provides a good question to think about: Does learning about cultures improve cultural sensitivity, or does it strengthen cultural stereotyping?

On one hand, we truly need to understand how one culture differs from another, so that we can identify cultural influences on our behaviors, develop an appreciation of different worldviews, and be attentive towards judgments and attribution bias. For example, for a German expatriating to Japan it would be useful to know about the differences in the preference for high versus low context communications between the two cultures. It is likely that a direct and task-oriented approach common in Germany would be considered rude in relationship-oriented Japan where saving face is important.

On the other hand, would such knowledge not also support a stereotyped difference between Western and Eastern cultures? Is it necessarily true and essential that Westerners are more direct, all of them, as a group? Stemming from previous research on the topic, Buchtel argues that cultural learning might legitimize stereotyping and support prejudices, as it increases the tendency for group categorization. This might result in a negative outcome, given that group categorization links to ‘US’ vs ‘THEM’ thinking, which in turn does the opposite of uniting people.

Carried out with students in a Cultural Psychology course, Buchtel’s study found that students, who took the course, were more able to accurately perceive cultural aspects of interpersonal interactions (metacognitive CQ), more conscious of culture in interpersonal interactions, and more non-judgmental than students in the control group. Although these results reveal that the effects of the cultural psychology course were more positive than negative, we should be cautious in drawing overly positive conclusions. Firstly, Buchtel also found that in the case of less motivated students, who did not perform well in this course, the course increased their endorsement of unconfirmed cultural stereotypes. In other words, this evidence suggests that if not taken with full responsibility and engagement by the learner, cultural learning may enhance stereotyping. Second, the researcher found that compared to the control group, after the Cultural Psychology course students did not improve on their self-reported knowledge about other cultures (cognitive CQ); confidence to deal with new cultures (motivational CQ); or adjustment of one’s behavior when interacting within different cultural contexts (behavioral CQ). As such, this specific course improved only one aspect of cultural intelligence – the metacognitive dimension, which refers to an individual’s cultural consciousness and awareness during intercultural contact.

What to make out of it in terms of global mobility?

I believe that this study is a good illustration and starting point for considering potential unexpected effects of cultural learning. We always aim for improving cultural competence, but in reality we risk having not only positive but also (or only) negative effects of such initiatives. As Buchtel concludes, ‘cultural differences should be discussed, but care must be taken to teach students that the existence of valid sociotypes does not imply validity of all stereotypes, essentialist thinking about individuals or cultures, or prejudiced attitudes’. In other words, there are valid facts about cultural differences that are worth teaching. Yet, cultural learning programs should also consider more explicitly that different groups can have more similarities than differences, and that differences within a given group can be larger than differences between groups. These programs also need to emphasize that there are ‘true’ and ‘false’ (unconfirmed) stereotypes, and that the ‘true’ stereotypes serve some sort of cultural purpose (i.e., that another group is not just acting ‘weird’). Finally, cultural learning programs should go beyond metacognitive aspects and target the remaining aspects of participants’ cultural intelligence.

Further reading:

Buchtel, E. E. (2014). Cultural sensitivity or cultural stereotyping? Positive and negative effects of a cultural psychology class. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 39, 3, 40-52.

4 thoughts on “How Should We Learn about Cultural Differences?

  1. The study outcome does not really surprise me as developing intercultural competence is a process and in that process we have to deal with several issues one of which often involved stronger stereotypes and an emotional reaction to cultural differences. I often warn my clients that a “defensive” (we are better, or our way is more sophisticated) reaction is common after an initial intervention when people have little to no knowledge about cultural differences. Key is not to stop there. I find working with the Developmental Model of Intercultural Sensitivity (DMIS) by Prof. Milton Bennett to design my interventions and facilitation very useful and effective. Hence, I was wondering if only the motivation of the students may have been an issue or if they may have started with a less complex experience of cultural differences than the other students? In DMIS terms they would have experiences in the Denial range and moved towards Defense while the other students moved from Defense to Minimization? Warm regards from Brazil, Elisabeth

  2. I have done a lot of research on cultural/national stereotypes. The reality is that we cannot live without these generalizations. It is impossible to talk about a world of billions of individuals, but we can talk about a world made up of diverse groups that share caracteristics. I always begin any cross-cultural training explaining how generalizations are necessary, but always potentially dangerous. I train individuals to be ready for potential differences that are true about the new group, but I am always stressing that every person they meet with be an exception on many levels, just are he/she is not a typical X (fill in the blank with own culture).

Leave a Reply to jdahl Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published.